Presumptive Democratic presidential nominee Hillary Clinton was in Springfield, Ill., Wednesday where she sought to use the symbolism of a historic landmark to draw parallels to a present-day America that is in need of repairing deepening racial and cultural divides.

The Old State Capitol — where Abraham Lincoln delivered his famous "A house divided" speech in 1858 warning against the ills of slavery and where Barack Obama launched his presidential bid in 2007 — served as the backdrop for Clinton as she spoke of how "America's long struggle with race is far from finished."

Episode 711: Hooked on Heroin

1 hour ago

When we meet the heroin dealer called Bone, he has just shot up. He has a lot to say anyway. He tells us about his career--it pretty much tracks the evolution of drug use in America these past ten years or so. He tells us about his rough past. And he tells us about how he died a week ago. He overdosed on his own supply and his friend took his body to the emergency room, then left.

New British Prime Minister Theresa May announced six members of her Cabinet Wednesday.

Amid a sweeping crackdown on dissent in Egypt, security forces have forcibly disappeared hundreds of people since the beginning of 2015, according to a new report from Amnesty International.

It's an "unprecedented spike," the group says, with an average of three or four people disappeared every day.

The Republican Party, as it prepares for its convention next week has checked off item No. 1 on its housekeeping list — drafting a party platform. The document reflects the conservative views of its authors, many of whom are party activists. So don't look for any concessions to changing views among the broader public on key social issues.

Many public figures who took to Twitter and Facebook following the murder of five police officers in Dallas have faced public blowback and, in some cases, found their employers less than forgiving about inflammatory and sometimes hateful online comments.

As Venezuela unravels — with shortages of food and medicine, as well as runaway inflation — President Nicolas Maduro is increasingly unpopular. But he's still holding onto power.

"The truth in Venezuela is there is real hunger. We are hungry," says a man who has invited me into his house in the northwestern city of Maracaibo, but doesn't want his name used for fear of reprisals by the government.

The wiry man paces angrily as he speaks. It wasn't always this way, he says, showing how loose his pants are now.

Ask a typical teenage girl about the latest slang and girl crushes and you might get answers like "spilling the tea" and Taylor Swift. But at the Girl Up Leadership Summit in Washington, D.C., the answers were "intersectional feminism" — the idea that there's no one-size-fits-all definition of feminism — and U.N. climate chief Christiana Figueres.

Copyright 2016 NPR. To see more, visit http://www.npr.org/.

Arizona Hispanics Poised To Swing State Blue

5 hours ago
Copyright 2016 NPR. To see more, visit http://www.npr.org/.

Pages

How Real Is The Candy Witch? Pretty Real, By Some Measures

Oct 28, 2013
Originally published on October 28, 2013 3:58 pm

Halloween is a time for creativity and imagination. Children and adults alike are given free rein to carve pumpkins, decorate wildly, conceive novel costumes and entertain witches and goblins and ghosts in their myriad forms.

For adults, the boundary between fantasy and reality is typically a clear one, at least when it comes to Halloween beasties. Flying witches are fantasy, but the 3-foot-tall "ghost" demanding candy on your doorstep on Oct. 31 is an all-too-real child beneath that white sheet.

But what about children? What do they make of Halloween's fantastical creatures? After all, many believe in Santa Claus and the Tooth Fairy. Are the fiends and other fictions of Halloween any different?

If you listen to folks like Richard Dawkins or Jerry Coyne, you might think children are the ultimate suckers, lulled into gullibility by evolutionary mechanisms, an "evolved credulity," that makes them believe whatever their elders tell them — whether it's that Santa Claus exists or that there's an omniscient creator.

Yet decades of research in developmental psychology paint a more nuanced picture of fantastical beliefs in childhood.

Children are generally pretty sophisticated when it comes to differentiating fantasy from reality, even though they often have rich fantasy lives populated by imaginary friends, fueled by fictional stories, and used to generate the diverse make-believe worlds that form the backdrop to imaginative play and pretense.

As early as preschool, children begin to understand that appearances can be misleading, and judge — for example — that a horse wearing a very convincing zebra costume is "really" a horse.

In fact, research suggests that children can be pretty skeptical at a young age. Belief in fantastical beings like Santa Claus seems to peak around age 4 and to decline by age 8, often in response to worries about how, for example, Santa could possibly make it to all those houses (and down all those chimneys after eating all those cookies) in one night. While children do typically treat their parents as reliable sources of information, they also assess how ideas fit with other things they believe about the world before taking them on board.

So where do children's beliefs in fictional beings come from? And does their receptivity to the reality of such beings change in the course of development?

To study the origins of belief in fantastical cultural creations, three researchers at the University of Texas at Austin — Jacqueline Woolley, Elizabeth Boerger and Arthur Markman — made up their very own fictional being: the Candy Witch. In a clever and well-known study, they tested whether and how preschool-aged children would come to believe she was real.

The Candy Witch was introduced to children at their childcare center just before Halloween. Children learned about the Candy Witch — depicted as a rosy-cheeked blonde woman (i.e., a poor candidate for Huffington Post's recent enumeration of the 13 most badass witches) — without explicitly being told she was real or pretend. Instead, a teacher explained that when she's invited to do so, the Candy Witch goes to children's houses on Halloween night and swaps some of their Halloween candy for a toy. The children later participated in an activity to make a Candy Witch puppet, the sort of project that children might complete around Easter (for the Easter Bunny) or Christmas (for Santa Claus).

Importantly, the researchers also varied whether the children received more direct evidence to support the existence of the Candy Witch. About half the children "overheard" their parents call the Candy Witch by phone on Halloween to make arrangements, and those same children found that the next morning, some of their candy had been swapped for a toy.

Of key interest were whether children would come to believe in the Candy Witch at all, whether their "gullibility" would decrease with age, and whether they'd be more likely to believe the Candy Witch was real after experiencing a candy-swap firsthand.

Of the complete set of 44 children in the study, 29 (66 percent) claimed that the Candy Witch was real just after Halloween. Almost a year later, 14 of the 22 children (64 percent) who were asked again claimed the Candy Witch was real, despite — I presume — having encountered no further references to her existence.

So the researchers were able to generate belief in a fantastical being among a majority of children. It surely helped that the teachers and parents who introduced the Candy Witch were sources the children already trusted, and that the Candy Witch was consistent with many of the children's existing beliefs, involving a mix of mundane and magical properties, much like the Tooth Fairy, and some relationship to a yearly event, like the Easter Bunny and Santa Claus.

Surprisingly, however, younger children (mostly 3-year-olds) were no more likely to be duped than older children (mostly 4- and 5-year-olds). In fact, the children who expressed the very strongest belief in the Candy Witch were older children who had received evidence for the Candy Witch in their homes. Among the older children, those whose parents had made the fake phone call and swapped candy for toys were more likely to endorse the existence of the Candy Witch than those whose parents had not, with no comparable effect for the younger kids. So children's willingness to believe in fantastical beings didn't simply decrease with age, but they did become more sensitive to the nature and strength of the evidence available.

A followup study with children as old as 7, conducted a few years later, found similar results: A high proportion of children became convinced that the Candy Witch is real, this belief often persisted one year later, and older children were no less likely to endorse the reality of the Candy Witch than were younger children. However, older children were more influenced by whether they had received direct evidence in the form of a Candy Witch visit at home, as in the study with preschoolers.

So, young children aren't so hard to fool when they receive sufficiently plausible information from reliable informants, but they aren't passive sponges that soak up whatever they're told, either. Not all children came to believe in the Candy Witch and, among older children who did, the nature of the evidence was important.

What does this all mean for Halloween? Well, you probably don't need to worry too much about the neighborhood kids mistaking you for a genuine penguin on the night. But, if you want to trick your own children out of some of their sugary treats, there's a good chance you can convince them the Candy Witch is ready to swap Halloween booty for new toys.


You can keep up with more of what Tania Lombrozo is thinking on Twitter: @TaniaLombrozo

Copyright 2013 NPR. To see more, visit http://www.npr.org/.